Academic Fraud
Thirty years
ago I wrote an unpublished medical novel, White
Squirrel, some of which is in the process of being reworked into my third
novel, The Dana Twins and Other Lies.
One story line
from White Squirrel which will not be
worked into The Dana Twins and Other Lies
involved academic fraud. When I set out to write fiction after a thirty
year hiatus, I wondered whether or not this theme would resonate with readers
of a novel, and decided that it might not.
The story of a
drug company or a manufacturer falsifying research to make millions has become a
staple of modern crime shows. Typically, in those dramas, a whistle blower is
murdered to conceal the truth and the investigator-protagonist steps in at that
point. Since the fraud involves huge amounts of money, the dishonesty is easily
understood by even the most unsophisticated viewer.
Academic
fraud, on the other hand, occurs on a much smaller scale. Real science is built
in small pieces, one brick at a time, and the stakes in academic fraud are
usually much smaller. Academic fraud often involves someone falsifying science
in order to generate a series of publications, or to advance their career –
gain tenure, or to please a mentor. To my knowledge, no one has ever been
killed to conceal academic fraud, though lives and careers have been destroyed.
The following tales happened approximately thirty years ago, and while they
were known in the academic community at that time, they have faded into
obscurity and a retelling seems appropriate, certainly more appropriate than
creating a fictional fraud just to write a novel about it.
A preliminary
investigation at Harvard found nothing out of the ordinary, except for what was
apparently a single aberrant action. However, when the overall body of Darsee’s
data was found to be inconsistent with results obtained by other investigators
working on the same project, Harvard was criticized for claiming to have
completely reviewed data which were – in essence – non-existent.
By the time
the entire sham was discovered, Dr. Darsee had been offered a faculty position at
Harvard based on his productivity. That offer was rescinded. Darsee was banned
from receiving NIH research funding for a period of ten years. He left
research, took a fellowship in critical care medicine, and, as best I could
learn, is in practice as an internist in Indianapolis, Indiana.
One of the
interesting themes of the Darsee story is that internal investigations are
often flawed by the fact that the people who do the investigating are the
people at the institution who are most familiar with the research issue in
question, i.e. the people who should have been supervising the study. Finding a
major problem in such a review would mean finding oneself guilty of not
providing adequate supervision. Hence, since one is, in essence, investigating
oneself, there is a vested interest in not finding academic fraud. That theme repeats
itself in other stories of scientific fraud. Ironically, a NY Times article
from December 15, 1981 praised Dr. Eugene Braunwald, Dr. Darsee’s supervisor at
Harvard, for acting with speed and responsibility to perform a thorough
investigation. That, of course, was before all the facts were known. At that
time, the investigation had uncovered only the single aberrant act by Darsee at
Harvard and had reached the conclusion that Darsee was a prolific and brilliant
researcher whose work at Emory was not in question. That latter statement was
true only until someone thought to look at Darsee’s previous work at Emory.
Overall, it seems that Darsee learned at an early point in his career that it
was easier to publish research data if one omitted the tedious step of actually
doing the research.
At the end of
the Darsee affair, numerous papers from Harvard and Emory were retracted.
Darsee’s supervisors were criticized for providing lax supervision, and the
prestigious New England Journal of Medicine took the opportunity to criticize
Darsee’s co-authors for attaching their names to work with which they were
unfamiliar. While Darsee’s chicanery was not the only force behind a greater
concern for assigning authorship on scientific papers, it was one of them.
Perhaps the
most prominent fraud in medical science involved Dr. William Summerlin, working
in the lab of the noted immunologist Dr. Robert Good, a scientist who had been
on the cover of TIME magazine in March of 1973.
In 1973, Dr. Summerlin
claimed that by growing cells in tissue culture he could graft incompatible
cells into mice, the most dramatic example being a graft of cells from a black
mouse into a white mouse. The work appeared to revolutionize transplant
immunology. I remember being at the national presentation of Dr. Summerlin’s
work in Atlantic City when my program director said, “That’s the most amazing
thing I ever heard – if it’s true.”
The house of
cards collapsed when an animal caretaker examining one of Summerlin’s mice
found that the black color could be washed off with alcohol.
As documented
in the book The Patchwork Mouse, Summerlin
was involved in a series of “interesting” experiments. For example, corneas can
be transplanted from one individual to another because the site is protected
from the immune system. This is not true when the corneal transplant involves
the limbus, the area where the cornea joins the sclera. In addition to the
black/white mouse experiments, Summerlin was running a study in which one eye of
a dog allegedly had a corneal transplant involving the limbus without the
tissue culture step. As expected that eye became cloudy. The other eye, the
control, had a limbus type corneal transplant, allegedly with the tissue
culture step. That eye was clear. There was just one problem. The clear eye was
actually an eye that had not had any surgery and the cloudy eye was an eye that
had undergone a transplant. In essence, Summerlin’s entire body of work was apparently
fraudulent.
Summerlin’s
background was in dermatology, not immunology, and he claimed that he was
“under pressure” to produce good results. One is left to wonder why such an
important group of experiments was entrusted to a dermatologist without formal
training in immunology.
What I have
found most interesting is the scene described by the authors of The Patchwork Mouse in which one of the
animal caretakers brought the mouse to a young surgeon at Sloan-Kettering and
showed him how the color “washed off.” Without hesitation, the surgeon, one of
my medical school classmates, Dr. John Raaf, brought the animal to Dr. Good’s
office. Informed by a secretary that Dr. Good was meeting with the Board of
Directors, or some equally important group of functionaries, Dr. Raaf allegedly
responded, “Get him out of there; this is important.”
The Summerlin story
illustrates the critical point that if you give too much responsibility to an
untrained individual, he may have to choose between failing or fabricating data.
He may choose the latter option. That theme is echoed in the story below.
The last tale
of academic fraud that I wish to share is my favorite because, as the story has
been reported, no one listened to the female whistleblower. Thus, the tale is
not only one of academic fraud, but also a case of the “old boy’s network”
versus a woman who was intellectually wronged. I personally think that this
tale has potential as a stage play. Unfortunately, as the case played out in
real life, the ending was too abrupt to be optimally dramatic, but, with a
little literary license, it might work.
In essence,
the story is this. Dr. Helena Washslicht-Rodbard (hereafter referred to as Dr.
Rodbard) and her supervisor, Dr. Jesse Roth, at the NIH performed a study
measuring insulin receptors in women with anorexia. The women were restudied
after they had been treated with behavioral therapy and had clinically
improved. The details and the results of the study are not critical to this
discussion.
The Rodbard
and Roth paper was submitted to the New England Journal of Medicine (NEJM). As
is the policy of all journals, the NEJM sent the paper to reviewers, in this
case three reviewers. One of the reviewers was Dr. Phil Felig of Yale, who, as
per relatively standard practice, gave the paper to Dr. Vijay Soman, an
investigator in his lab, to review.
Dr. Soman
recommended rejection of the Rodbard and Roth paper; the other two reviewers
recommended acceptance after revision. Shortly thereafter, Soman and Felig
submitted a similar paper to the American Journal of Medicine and, since the
work was in a somewhat esoteric field, it was sent to Dr. Roth to review. As
might be expected, he shared the manuscript with Dr. Rodbard, and a major
scandal was brewing.
Dr. Rodbard
immediately recognized that the Methods Section of the paper seemed to be taken
word for word from her manuscript. Obviously, there was an issue of plagiarism.
Since the methods section tells how something was done, the excuse was offered
at some point, that Dr. Soman “borrowed” Dr. Rodbard’s description of her
techniques since they had done the same thing and her description was
“perfect.” The second issue was one of propriety. Dr. Rodbard guessed,
correctly, that since Soman and Felig had “borrowed” her Methods Section, they
had, obviously, seen her paper. Clearly, they had been one of the reviewers. In
fact, they had given the review which recommended rejection to the Rodbard and
Roth paper which presented work similar to their own work.
In science,
primary discovery, even in a very limited field, has intellectual if not
monetary value. By recommending rejection of the Rodbard and Roth paper, Soman
and Felig increased the chance that their work would be published first. The
question was raised as to whether Soman and Felig should have notified the
editors at NEJM of this potential conflict. Since Felig (at Yale) and Roth (at
the NIH) were long-time friends, (not only professional acquaintances, but
actually boyhood friends), they agreed to meet and review the two papers. When
that was done, the plagiarism was obvious. To resolve the problem, an agreement
was worked out whereby priority would be resolved in favor of Rodbard and Roth.
The Rodbard and Roth paper would be published; the Soman and Felig paper would
be published after Rodbard and Roth had published, and it would cite the work
by Rodbard and Roth.
Things might
have ended simply at this point, except for one thing. On further review, Dr.
Rodbard became suspicious that Dr. Soman had done more than “borrow” her
methods section. Her suspicions had a major impact on the careers of several
individuals and on two major academic institutions.
Both the paper by Rodbard and Roth and the paper by Felig and Soman studied anorexic women at baseline and after their anorexia had
improved. The subjects at the NIH had been part of an intense psychotherapy
program which was responsible for their clinical improvement. The therapist was
a co-author on Rodbard and Roth’s paper. Soman and
Felig’s subjects had “improved” to a similar degree. However no statement was
made as to what had been done from a psychiatric point of view to lead to the
improvement. Also, there was no co-author from the Department of Psychiatry on their
paper. Dr. Rodbard brought her concerns to a Dean at Yale and also to the New
England Journal of Medicine.
The Dean at
Yale (Dr. Robert Berliner) accepted reassurances from Dr. Felig that all was
well with the work published under his name and Dr. Soman’s name. The Dean informed
Dr. Rodbard of that fact. Not surprisingly, Dr. Rodbard was less than pleased with the results of
the investigation.
Somewhere
during the process, Dr. Rodbard’s supervisor (and co-author), Dr. Roth, essentially
told her to cease and desist, and not to use NIH stationery in promulgating her
complaints – which he seemed to regard as a personal vendetta. There is of
course no way to know how his opinion was biased by his long term friendship
with Dr. Felig at Yale.
When I write fiction,
I tend to see scenes, and I imagine Dr. Roth telling Dr. Rodbard to calm down,
and Dr. Rodbard replying that important principles were involved and that
instead of her calming down, he should be getting excited. Considering that he
is in a senior position, and male, and she is the junior person and female, one
can imagine the rest of the dialogue on one’s own.
In any case, with
a controversy smoldering, and legitimate questions about the adequacy of the
Yale internal review, the New England Journal of Medicine sent an associate
editor (Dr. Jeffrey Flier) to Yale to interview Dr. Soman, the junior faculty
member who had seemingly duplicated the work of Rodbard and Roth. Almost as
soon as Dr. Flier began his review, he became suspicious of Dr. Soman’s work.
Data appeared to be missing, and where there was data, it looked too good to be
true. Stated simply, in laboratory studies, points may lie near a straight
line, but not exactly "on the line."
Dr. Flier
challenged Dr. Soman about the data and Dr. Soman quickly confessed that
he had “made up” some of the results due to a tremendous pressure to produce
results and publish quickly. Flier had uncovered in a few hours what an internal Yale review had failed to find in a year.
The fallout
from the fraud brought the scandal to the public eye, leading to the telling of
the tale in the prestigious journal Science.
Prior to Soman’s confessing to the fraud, Dr. Felig, who was Chief of
Endocrinology at Yale, had been hired as Chief of Medicine at Columbia. Felig had recommended
that Dr. Soman be appointed a member of the faculty at Columbia. With the fraud
revealed, Felig informed Columbia about what had happened and told Columbia that
Dr. Soman could not be offered a position. He did not mention, at that time,
the failure of the previous internal Yale investigation to uncover the fraud.
When all the
facts came to light, Columbia became aware that an outside auditor quickly found what the internal Yale review (by Dr. Felig and one of the Yale
Deans) had not uncovered. Dr. Felig was asked to resign as Chair of
Medicine at Columbia since it was felt that the entire affair tarnished his
ability to lead by example. Dr. Soman had, by this time, resigned from his
position at Yale and returned to his native India. Twelve papers that Dr. Soman had
published, eight of which had been co-authored by Dr. Felig, were retracted. Dr.
Felig returned to Yale as a tenured Professor but without a named chair. (Since
I am a professor, but do not have a named chair and do not aspire to one, I
cannot say how much of a penalty that represents.)
Dr. Rodbard
left the NIH. However, she was, as best as I can determine, in the midst of her training and not in a permanent
position. She went into private practice in endocrinology. When I
taught about academic fraud in the History of Medicine course at Vanderbilt,
Dr. Rodbard was one of the heroes and I was happy to learn that in
the intervening decades she went on to serve as president of both the American
College of Endocrinology and the American Association of Clinical
Endocrinologists.
Dr. Jeffrey Flier, the external investigator who uncovered
the fraud, also has had a distinguished career. He went on the become Dean of the Faculty at Harvard Medical School.
The cases of Dr.
Summerlin and Dr. Soman are examples of a junior person functioning without
adequate supervision and illustrate why internal reviews often fail to
detect obvious problems. Whether academic fraud is more common or less common
now as compared to thirty years ago is unknown. Academic fraud is hard to measure.
People have attempted to
measure academic fraud by counting "results which cannot be replicated."
However, as scientists know, we generally take a probability level of 0.05 (1
in 20) as a level of statistical significance. This means that if a finding is
positive at a level of 0.05, there is a 1 in 20 chance that the finding is
random. Failure to replicate a result does not mean that something untoward was
involved. It may simply mean that the initial observation was random.
We do know
that academic fraud has not disappeared. Fifteen years ago, the most common
condition for which a stem cell transplant was done was breast cancer. The
value of the procedure was unclear as all of the studies showing a benefit for
high dose chemotherapy in conjunction with stem cell transplant involved
historical controls which were not truly similar to the patients undergoing the
transplant procedure. Since there was legitimate concern over the value of the
procedure, several randomized concurrent studies were performed to demonstrate
whether or not stem cell transplant in breast cancer was of clinical value. All
but one of the studies showed no benefit; the doctor conducting the study
showing a positive result confessed to manipulating his data.